Näytä suppeat kuvailutiedot

dc.contributor.authorUmbers, Kate D. L.
dc.contributor.authorWhite, Thomas E.
dc.contributor.authorDe Bona, Sebastiano
dc.contributor.authorHaff, Tonya
dc.contributor.authorRyeland, Julia
dc.contributor.authorDrinkwater, Eleanor
dc.contributor.authorMappes, Johanna
dc.date.accessioned2019-02-11T10:26:45Z
dc.date.available2019-02-11T10:26:45Z
dc.date.issued2019
dc.identifier.citationUmbers, K. D. L., White, T. E., De Bona, S., Haff, T., Ryeland, J., Drinkwater, E., & Mappes, J. (2019). The protective value of a defensive display varies with the experience of wild predators. <i>Scientific Reports</i>, <i>9</i>, Article 463. <a href="https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-36995-9" target="_blank">https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-36995-9</a>
dc.identifier.otherCONVID_28886920
dc.identifier.otherTUTKAID_80460
dc.identifier.urihttps://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/62741
dc.description.abstractPredation has driven the evolution of diverse adaptations for defence among prey, and one striking example is the deimatic display. While such displays can resemble, or indeed co-occur with, aposematic ‘warning’ signals, theory suggests deimatic displays may function independently of predator learning. The survival value of deimatic displays against wild predators has not been tested before. Here we used the mountain katydid Acripeza reticulata to test the efficacy of a putative deimatic display in the wild. Mountain katydids have a complex defence strategy; they are camouflaged at rest, but reveal a striking red-, blue-, and black-banded abdomen when attacked. We presented live katydids to sympatric (experienced) and allopatric (naive) natural predators, the Australian magpie Cracticus tibicen, and observed bird reactions and katydid behaviors and survival during repeated interactions. The efficacy of the katydids’ defence differed with predator experience. Their survival was greatest when faced with naïve predators, which provided clear evidence of the protective value of the display. In contrast, katydid survival was consistently less likely when facing experienced predators. Our results suggest that sympatric predators have learned to attack and consume mountain katydids despite their complex defense, and that their post-attack display can be an effective deterrent, particularly against naïve predators. These results suggest that deimatism does not require predator learning to afford protection, but that a predator can learn to expect the display and subsequently avoid it or ignore it. That sympatric predators learn to ignore the defense is a possible explanation for the mountain katydid’s counter-intuitive behavior of revealing warning colors only after tactile stimuli from predator attack.fi
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherNature Publishing Group
dc.relation.ispartofseriesScientific Reports
dc.rightsCC BY 4.0
dc.subject.otherbehavioural ecology
dc.subject.otherdefensive display
dc.subject.otherkatydids
dc.titleThe protective value of a defensive display varies with the experience of wild predators
dc.typearticle
dc.identifier.urnURN:NBN:fi:jyu-201901251308
dc.contributor.laitosBio- ja ympäristötieteiden laitosfi
dc.contributor.laitosDepartment of Biological and Environmental Scienceen
dc.contributor.oppiaineEkologia ja evoluutiobiologiafi
dc.contributor.oppiaineBiologisten vuorovaikutusten huippututkimusyksikköfi
dc.contributor.oppiaineEcology and Evolutionary Biologyen
dc.contributor.oppiaineCentre of Excellence in Biological Interactions Researchen
dc.type.urihttp://purl.org/eprint/type/JournalArticle
dc.date.updated2019-01-25T10:15:14Z
dc.type.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.description.reviewstatuspeerReviewed
dc.relation.issn2045-2322
dc.relation.numberinseries0
dc.relation.volume9
dc.type.versionpublishedVersion
dc.rights.copyright© The Authors 2019
dc.rights.accesslevelopenAccessfi
dc.relation.grantnumber284666
dc.subject.ysopuolustusmekanismit (biologia)
dc.subject.ysohepokatit
dc.subject.ysoeläinten käyttäytyminen
dc.format.contentfulltext
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p6078
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p21975
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p18481
dc.rights.urlhttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
dc.relation.doi10.1038/s41598-018-36995-9
dc.relation.funderSuomen Akatemiafi
dc.relation.funderResearch Council of Finlanden
jyx.fundingprogramHuippuyksikkörahoitus, SAfi
jyx.fundingprogramCentre of Excellence, AoFen
jyx.fundinginformationK.U. was supported by the Hermon Slade Foundation, Western Sydney University, and the NSW Linnean Society. S.D.B. and J.M. were supported by the Hermon Slade Foundation and the Centre of Excellence in Biological Interactions of the Academy of Finland, J.R. and E.D. were supported by the Hermon Slade Foundation. We thank Giselle Muschett, Nola Umbers, Mark Umbers, Farley Lanfear, Rob Lanfear, Nyah Lanfear, Rachael Dudaniec, and Stephanie Martin for their fieldwork assistance, Alex Barley for compiling the supplementary video, and Tim Kahlke for his expertise in katydid husbandry. We also thank Redbank lodge and Thredbo Sports for their support during fieldwork. Finally, we thank Michael R. Whitehead for permission to use his photograph of the mountain katydid. Hermon Slade Foundation, Western Sydney University
dc.type.okmA1


Aineistoon kuuluvat tiedostot

Thumbnail

Aineisto kuuluu seuraaviin kokoelmiin

Näytä suppeat kuvailutiedot

CC BY 4.0
Ellei muuten mainita, aineiston lisenssi on CC BY 4.0