dc.contributor.author | Salakka, Ilja | |
dc.contributor.author | Pitkäniemi, Anni | |
dc.contributor.author | Pentikäinen, Emmi | |
dc.contributor.author | Mikkonen, Kari | |
dc.contributor.author | Saari, Pasi | |
dc.contributor.author | Toiviainen, Petri | |
dc.contributor.author | Särkämö, Teppo | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2021-05-18T04:40:24Z | |
dc.date.available | 2021-05-18T04:40:24Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2021 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Salakka, I., Pitkäniemi, A., Pentikäinen, E., Mikkonen, K., Saari, P., Toiviainen, P., & Särkämö, T. (2021). What makes music memorable? : Relationships between acoustic musical features and music-evoked emotions and memories in older adults. <i>PLoS ONE</i>, <i>16</i>(5), Article e0251692. <a href="https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251692" target="_blank">https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251692</a> | |
dc.identifier.other | CONVID_73075107 | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/75697 | |
dc.description.abstract | Background and objectives
Music has a unique capacity to evoke both strong emotions and vivid autobiographical memories. Previous music information retrieval (MIR) studies have shown that the emotional experience of music is influenced by a combination of musical features, including tonal, rhythmic, and loudness features. Here, our aim was to explore the relationship between music-evoked emotions and music-evoked memories and how musical features (derived with MIR) can predict them both.
Methods
Healthy older adults (N = 113, age ≥ 60 years) participated in a listening task in which they rated a total of 140 song excerpts comprising folk songs and popular songs from 1950s to 1980s on five domains measuring the emotional (valence, arousal, emotional intensity) and memory (familiarity, autobiographical salience) experience of the songs. A set of 24 musical features were extracted from the songs using computational MIR methods. Principal component analyses were applied to reduce multicollinearity, resulting in six core musical components, which were then used to predict the behavioural ratings in multiple regression analyses.
Results
All correlations between behavioural ratings were positive and ranged from moderate to very high (r = 0.46–0.92). Emotional intensity showed the highest correlation to both autobiographical salience and familiarity. In the MIR data, three musical components measuring salience of the musical pulse (Pulse strength), relative strength of high harmonics (Brightness), and fluctuation in the frequencies between 200–800 Hz (Low-mid) predicted both music-evoked emotions and memories. Emotional intensity (and valence to a lesser extent) mediated the predictive effect of the musical components on music-evoked memories.
Conclusions
The results suggest that music-evoked emotions are strongly related to music-evoked memories in healthy older adults and that both music-evoked emotions and memories are predicted by the same core musical features. | en |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | |
dc.language.iso | eng | |
dc.publisher | Public Library of Science (PLoS) | |
dc.relation.ispartofseries | PLoS ONE | |
dc.rights | CC BY 4.0 | |
dc.subject.other | emotions | |
dc.subject.other | music cognition | |
dc.subject.other | music perception | |
dc.subject.other | memory | |
dc.subject.other | bioacoustics | |
dc.subject.other | elderly | |
dc.subject.other | entropy | |
dc.subject.other | regression analysis | |
dc.title | What makes music memorable? : Relationships between acoustic musical features and music-evoked emotions and memories in older adults | |
dc.type | article | |
dc.identifier.urn | URN:NBN:fi:jyu-202105182968 | |
dc.contributor.laitos | Musiikin, taiteen ja kulttuurin tutkimuksen laitos | fi |
dc.contributor.laitos | Department of Music, Art and Culture Studies | en |
dc.contributor.oppiaine | Musiikkitiede | fi |
dc.contributor.oppiaine | Musicology | en |
dc.type.uri | http://purl.org/eprint/type/JournalArticle | |
dc.type.coar | http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1 | |
dc.description.reviewstatus | peerReviewed | |
dc.relation.issn | 1932-6203 | |
dc.relation.numberinseries | 5 | |
dc.relation.volume | 16 | |
dc.type.version | publishedVersion | |
dc.rights.copyright | © 2021 Salakka et al. | |
dc.rights.accesslevel | openAccess | fi |
dc.subject.yso | tunteet | |
dc.subject.yso | muisti (kognitio) | |
dc.subject.yso | musiikkipsykologia | |
dc.subject.yso | ikääntyneet | |
dc.subject.yso | muistot | |
dc.subject.yso | musiikki | |
dc.format.content | fulltext | |
jyx.subject.uri | http://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p3485 | |
jyx.subject.uri | http://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p2607 | |
jyx.subject.uri | http://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p13805 | |
jyx.subject.uri | http://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p2433 | |
jyx.subject.uri | http://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p13859 | |
jyx.subject.uri | http://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p1808 | |
dc.rights.url | https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ | |
dc.relation.doi | 10.1371/journal.pone.0251692 | |
jyx.fundinginformation | Financial support for the work was provided by the Academy of Finland (grants 299044, 305264, 306625, and 327996) and the European Research Council (ERC-StG grant 803466). | |
dc.type.okm | A1 | |