Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorvan Bergen, Elsje
dc.contributor.authorVasalampi, Kati
dc.contributor.authorTorppa, Minna
dc.date.accessioned2020-03-31T10:40:24Z
dc.date.available2020-03-31T10:40:24Z
dc.date.issued2021
dc.identifier.citationvan Bergen, E., Vasalampi, K., & Torppa, M. (2021). How Are Practice and Performance Related? Development of Reading From Age 5 to 15. <i>Reading Research Quarterly</i>, <i>56</i>(3), 415-434. <a href="https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.309" target="_blank">https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.309</a>
dc.identifier.otherCONVID_35142109
dc.identifier.urihttps://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/68400
dc.description.abstractDoes reading a lot lead to better reading skills, or does reading a lot follow from high initial reading skills? The authors present a longitudinal study of how much children choose to read and how well they decode and comprehend texts. This is the first study to examine the codevelopment of print exposure with both fluency and comprehension throughout childhood using autocorrelations. Print exposure was operationalized as children’s amount of independent reading for pleasure. Two hundred children were followed from age 5 to age 15. Print exposure was assessed at ages 5, 7, 8, 9, and 13. Prereading skills were tested at age 5 and reading skills at ages 7, 8, 9, 14, and 15 (the latter with the Programme for International Student Assessment [PISA]). Before children learned to read (i.e., age 5), prereading skills and print exposure were not linked. Path analyses showed that children’s print exposure and reading skills reciprocally influence each other. During the early school years, the effects run from reading fluency to comprehension and print exposure, so from skills to amount. The effect of accumulated practice only emerged in adolescence. Reading fluency, comprehension, and print exposure were all important predictors of age 15 PISA reading comprehension. These findings were largely confirmed by post hoc models with random intercepts. Because foundational reading skills predicted changes in later reading comprehension and print exposure, the authors speculate that intervening decoding difficulties may positively impact exposure to and comprehension of texts. How much children read seems to matter most after the shift from learning to read to reading to learn.en
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.languageeng
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherJohn Wiley & Sons
dc.relation.ispartofseriesReading Research Quarterly
dc.rightsCC BY-NC-ND 4.0
dc.subject.otherdecoding
dc.subject.othercomprehension
dc.subject.othermotivation/engagement
dc.subject.otherfluency
dc.subject.otherdevelopmental theories
dc.subject.otherattitudes
dc.subject.otherchildren’s literature
dc.subject.otheremergent literacy
dc.subject.otherlongitudinal analysis
dc.subject.otherpath analysis
dc.titleHow Are Practice and Performance Related? Development of Reading From Age 5 to 15
dc.typearticle
dc.identifier.urnURN:NBN:fi:jyu-202003312614
dc.contributor.laitosOpettajankoulutuslaitosfi
dc.contributor.laitosKasvatustieteiden laitosfi
dc.contributor.laitosDepartment of Teacher Educationen
dc.contributor.laitosDepartment of Educationen
dc.contributor.oppiaineKasvatuspsykologiafi
dc.contributor.oppiainePsykologiafi
dc.contributor.oppiaineKasvatuspsykologiaen
dc.contributor.oppiainePsychologyen
dc.type.urihttp://purl.org/eprint/type/JournalArticle
dc.type.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.description.reviewstatuspeerReviewed
dc.format.pagerange415-434
dc.relation.issn1936-2722
dc.relation.numberinseries3
dc.relation.volume56
dc.type.versionpublishedVersion
dc.rights.copyright© 2020 The Authors
dc.rights.accesslevelopenAccessfi
dc.relation.grantnumber323773
dc.relation.grantnumber299506
dc.relation.grantnumber276239
dc.subject.ysositoutuminen (toiminta)
dc.subject.ysoluetun ymmärtäminen
dc.subject.ysolukeminen
dc.subject.ysopitkittäistutkimus
dc.subject.ysosujuvuus
dc.subject.ysomotivaatio
dc.subject.ysolapset (ikäryhmät)
dc.subject.ysolukuharrastus
dc.subject.ysoasenteet
dc.subject.ysolastenkirjallisuus
dc.subject.ysolukutaito
dc.format.contentfulltext
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p13419
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p20611
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p11406
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p14610
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p21865
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p4734
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p4354
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p27093
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p5619
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p4742
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p11405
dc.rights.urlhttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
dc.relation.doi10.1002/rrq.309
dc.relation.funderResearch Council of Finlanden
dc.relation.funderResearch Council of Finlanden
dc.relation.funderResearch Council of Finlanden
dc.relation.funderSuomen Akatemiafi
dc.relation.funderSuomen Akatemiafi
dc.relation.funderSuomen Akatemiafi
jyx.fundingprogramAcademy Research Fellow, AoFen
jyx.fundingprogramAcademy Project, AoFen
jyx.fundingprogramAcademy Research Fellow, AoFen
jyx.fundingprogramAkatemiatutkija, SAfi
jyx.fundingprogramAkatemiahanke, SAfi
jyx.fundingprogramAkatemiatutkija, SAfi
jyx.fundinginformationVan Bergen’s work was supported by a Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (NWO) Rubicon Fellowship (grant 446-12-005), a NWO Veni Fellow ship (grant 451-15-017), and a NWO Gravitation–funded Con sortium on Individual Development grant (024.001.003). Vasalampi’s work was supported by Academy of Finland grants (299506 and 323773). Torppa’s work was supported by an Academy of Finland Research Fellowship (grant 2762392).
dc.type.okmA1


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

CC BY-NC-ND 4.0
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as CC BY-NC-ND 4.0