Näytä suppeat kuvailutiedot

dc.contributor.authorHautala, Jarkko
dc.contributor.authorHyönä, Jukka
dc.contributor.authorAro, Mikko
dc.date.accessioned2011-11-24T13:51:43Z
dc.date.available2011-11-24T13:51:43Z
dc.date.issued2011
dc.identifier.citationHautala, J., Hyönä, J., & Aro, M. (2011). Dissociating spatial and letter-based word length effects observed in readers' eye movement patterns. <i>Vision Research</i>, <i>51</i>(15), 1719-1727. <a href="https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.05.015" target="_blank">https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.05.015</a>
dc.identifier.otherCONVID_20728992
dc.identifier.otherTUTKAID_46884
dc.identifier.urihttps://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/36996
dc.description.abstractIn previous eye movement research on word length effects, spatial width has been confounded with the number of letters. McDonald (2006) unconfounded these factors by rendering all words in sentences in constant spatial width. In the present study, the Arial font with proportional letter spacing was used for varying the number of letters while equating for spatial width, while the Courier font with monospaced letter spacing was used to measure the contribution of spatial width to the observed word length effect. Number of letters in words affected single fixation duration on target words, whereas words’ spatial width determined fixation locations in words and the probability of skipping a word. The results support the existence of distinct subsystems for deciding where and when to move eyes in text (Rayner & McConkie, 1976). The number-of-letters effect in fixation duration may be explained by visual acuity, visual crowding, and/or serial letter processing.
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherElsevier
dc.relation.ispartofseriesVision Research
dc.subject.othersanan pituus
dc.subject.otherkirjainten lukumäärä
dc.subject.othersanan spatiaalinen leveys
dc.subject.otherword length
dc.subject.othernumber of letters
dc.subject.otherspatial width
dc.titleDissociating spatial and letter-based word length effects observed in readers' eye movement patterns
dc.typearticle
dc.identifier.urnURN:NBN:fi:jyu-2011112211709
dc.contributor.laitosKasvatustieteiden laitosfi
dc.contributor.laitosPsykologian laitosfi
dc.contributor.laitosDepartment of Educationen
dc.contributor.laitosDepartment of Psychologyen
dc.contributor.oppiaineErityispedagogiikkafi
dc.contributor.oppiainePsykologiafi
dc.contributor.oppiaineSpecial Educationen
dc.contributor.oppiainePsychologyen
dc.type.urihttp://purl.org/eprint/type/JournalArticle
dc.date.updated2011-11-22T04:30:05Z
dc.type.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1
dc.description.reviewstatuspeerReviewed
dc.format.pagerange1719-1727
dc.relation.issn0042-6989
dc.relation.numberinseries15
dc.relation.volume51
dc.type.versionacceptedVersion
dc.rights.copyright© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. This is an electronic final draft version of an article whose final and defenitive form has been published in Vision Research by Elsevier.
dc.rights.accesslevelopenAccessfi
dc.subject.ysosilmänliikkeet
dc.subject.ysolukeminen
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p23744
jyx.subject.urihttp://www.yso.fi/onto/yso/p11406
dc.relation.doi10.1016/j.visres.2011.05.015
dc.type.okmA1


Aineistoon kuuluvat tiedostot

Thumbnail

Aineisto kuuluu seuraaviin kokoelmiin

Näytä suppeat kuvailutiedot